Brought to you by Newscorp Australia

How to tell if your dog is a genius

Jan Hoole, February 4, 2021 6:45PM Kids News

Print Article

A border collie pup. Collies are the breed most commonly used in studies of this type as they are bred to attend to audible commands and are very highly motivated to carry out tasks and to please the handler. media_cameraA border collie pup. Collies are the breed most commonly used in studies of this type as they are bred to attend to audible commands and are very highly motivated to carry out tasks and to please the handler.

animals

Reading level: orange

A simple experiment that’s easy to try at home with your own dog has shown that a few “talented” dogs are able to learn the name of a new object after only hearing it four times.

This ability was previously thought to be confined to humans.

The researchers found this ability was not common among all the dogs studied, instead it may be limited to a few “talented” or highly trained individuals.

The results of the study are published in the journal Nature.

So how can you tell whether your own dog is a genius or not?

Just follow the steps the researchers took to see whether your dog can learn the names of objects as quickly. But don’t worry if your dog doesn’t have this ability, it might just be down to their breed or previous experience.

Whisky the collie. Picture: Claudia Fugazza media_cameraWhisky the collie. Picture: Claudia Fugazza

WHISKY AND VICKY NINA
The study involved a collie called Whisky, who knew 59 objects by name, and a Yorkshire terrier called Vicky Nina, who knew 42 toys.

The researchers tested each dogs’ knowledge of their toys by asking them to bring each toy in turn. Neither the owners nor the experimenters could see the toys, to avoid influencing the dogs’ choice.

Once it was established the dogs knew the names of all their toys, the researchers introduced two new objects, placing each in turn in a group of known toys. In this test Whisky chose the new toy every single time. Vicky Nina fetched the right one in 52.5 per cent of trials, which is slightly above chance.

Vicky Nina with all her toys. Picture: Marco Ojeda media_cameraVicky Nina with all her toys. Picture: Marco Ojeda

LEARNING NEW NAMES
For the next part of the study the dog was shown a toy, told its name and was then allowed to play with it. After four repetitions of the name of two different new toys, the dog was asked to choose one of the two new toys.

No familiar toys were included in this part of the trial, to prevent the dog choosing the right toy by guessing the unfamiliar word must indicate the unfamiliar toy.

Both dogs chose the new toy more often than chance would predict, suggesting they were indeed learning the name of a new object very quickly. However, their memory decayed* considerably after 10 minutes and almost completely after one hour. This shows the new learning needs more reinforcement if it is to be retained*.

The test involving the new toy was also carried out by 20 volunteers with their own dogs, but these dogs didn’t show the ability to learn new names after few hearings.

The authors suggested the difference between the performance of the two dogs in their test and the volunteer dogs means, in order to learn new names quickly, the dog might need to be unusually intelligent or to have a lot of experience in learning names.

media_cameraA male border collie dog.

CLEVER DOGS
It seems likely there are a combination of factors at work in these experiments. It’s significant that the breed most commonly used in studies of this type is a border collie, which is bred to attend to audible* commands and is very highly motivated to carry out tasks and to please the handler. Yorkshire terriers also enjoy mental and physical stimulation.

Similar tests have been carried out by other research groups, usually using border collies. In 2004, a dog called Rico was found to know the names of 200 different objects, and in 2011 Chaser learnt 1022 unique objects.

Other breeds may simply be less interested in playing with or fetching toys. For example sight hounds, such as salukis and greyhounds, are primarily bred for hunting or racing, so are generally more difficult to train. They may show no interest in toys at all, as well as being considerably less motivated to please the handler.

Both the experimental dogs in this study were intensively trained, through play and social interaction, to pay attention to the names and characteristics of the toys. This might make them more likely to notice the differences between new and familiar toys, and to attend to the verbal cue* associated with them.

Although their training was not formal, it was nevertheless positive reinforcement training, a powerful method for teaching animals and humans. The dogs have undoubtedly learned their skills to a high degree.

It’s quite possible to teach all dogs to perform tasks, including learning the names of objects. But the degree to which they’re willing and able to learn, and to carry out the task, is very much regulated by breed of dog and the level of motivation the individual dog possesses.

If your pet is an Afghan hound or a St Bernard, you should not expect it to be interested in spending hours fetching toys for you. If, on the other hand, you have a border collie or a poodle, their abilities may only be limited by your imagination and your dedication to playing with them.

QLD_GCB_NEW_DOGS_18AUG15 media_cameraLollie the five-year-old St Bernard and Brynn the two-year-old Cavalier King Charles spaniel. Picture: Glenn Hampson

Jan Hoole is a Lecturer in Biology at Keele University, UK.
This article was originally published on The Conversation and is republished with permission.

GLOSSARY

  • decayed: reduced over time
  • retained: remembered
  • audible: able to be heard
  • verbal cue: spoken prompt or command

EXTRA READING

Aussie dogs to train as pandemic sniffer force

Australia’s biggest dog helping children read

Humans can’t resist those sad, puppy dog eyes

Body language study translates dog talk

QUICK QUIZ

  1. What animal is this study about?
  2. What did the researchers find out?
  3. Explain how they tested the study animals.
  4. Where can you read the results of the study?
  5. What breed of dog is most likely to succeed at this training? Why?

LISTEN TO THIS STORY

CLASSROOM ACTIVITIES
1. Design an Experiment
‘This research was a waste of time!’ Do you agree or disagree with this statement? Write a letter or post to Kids News explaining your opinion. Make your writing as persuasive as you can.

Time: allow 20 minutes to complete this activity
Curriculum Links: English, Science

2. Extension
Write a story that starts with: “I thought owning the world’s smartest dog would be easy, but …”

Time: allow at least 30 minutes to complete this activity
Curriculum Links: English

VCOP ACTIVITY
Stretch your sentence

Find a ‘who’ in the story, a person or animal.

Write it down.

Add 3 adjectives to describe them better.

Now add a verb to your list. What are they doing in the story?

Add an adverb about how they are doing the action.

Using all the words listed, create one descriptive sentence.

HAVE YOUR SAY: What’s the cleverest thing you’ve seen a dog do?
No one-word answers. Use full sentences to explain your thinking. No comments will be published until approved by editors.

Extra Reading in animals